THE SLEEP STAGES
THE SLEEP STAGES Initial discoveries
The next step in the evolution of understanding
sleep was the recognition that EEG brainwaves,
when combined with other kinds of physiological
information, could be used to identify rhythmically
recurring, discrete sleep stages. The first description
of these sleep stages was made by the American
scientist Alfred Loomis (1887–1975).
As a young man
in the army he developed the Aberdeen Chronograph,
a system for measuring projectile velocity by firing
a bullet through revolving paper-covered aluminum
disks. He went on to a successful career in investment
banking.
Becoming restless once again, and still
remembering his success with the chronograph, he
turned his attention to developing radar for military
purposes and for ground control during the landing
approach of aircraft. He was fascinated with the
measuring of waveforms and, among the many
projects at his laboratory at Tuxedo Park, New York,
was the study of sleep. Using a large 8 foot (2.4 m)
diameter recording drum, he described in 1937 a series of five discrete recurring sleep stages during
the night, which he rather unpoetically designated as
stages A–E.
In terms of later development, stages A
and B correspond roughly to what was later called
stage 1, C corresponded to stage 2, and D and E
resembled slow-wave sleep. All together they
correspond to what we now call non-rapid eye
movement (NREM) sleep.
The discovery of rapid eye movement (REM) sleep
The next big development, which in effect ushered
in the modern age of sleep research, occurred in
the early 1950s.
Nathaniel Kleitman (1895–1999), a
physiologist at the University of Chicago, had been
interested in eye movements and blinking as a
marker of sleep onset and depth of sleep, as well as
possible rhythmic behaviors, in infants. He enlisted
the aid of a graduate student, Eugene Aserinsky
(1921–98). Following observations in infants, they
adapted the technique of the electrooculogram (EOG)
for continuous use in sleep of children and adults.
In the process of doing so, they observed the periodic
appearance of vigorous and jerky ocular activity.
This new stage, known as rapid eye movement (REM)
sleep, was characterized not only by the eponymous
eye movements, but also by relaxation of the major
weight-bearing muscles, irregularity of respiratory
and heart rate, and loss of temperature control.
It also has psychological counterparts, and most
dreaming, in the conventional sense of the word,
occurs in REM. Indeed, REM sleep is as different from the rest of sleep (dubbed by Kleitman as
non-REM or NREM sleep) as NREM is from waking.
This has led some authors to describe humans as
having three distinct states of consciousness:
waking, REM, and NREM sleep.
The sleep stages do not appear randomly, but instead
are manifest in a rhythmic, repetitive pattern
throughout the night. There are a number of
influences on the appearance and duration of the individual stages, including a basic approximately
90–100-minute innate rhythmic cycle of NREM and
REM sleep (an example of “ultradian rhythms,” see
page 75), the time of the 24-hour day at which sleep
occurs, and the duration of wakefulness before sleep.
In the next section we will describe the sleep stages
in much more detail, but perhaps the important
message at this point is that sleep is not a unitary
process, but is comprised of two very distinct states
as well as several distinct stages in NREM sleep.
Post a Comment